Paper – Obrist et al. Opportunities for Odor: Experience with Smell and Implications for Technology

Opportunities for Odor:

Experiences with Smell and Implications for Technology

Marianna Obrist1,2, Alexandre N. Tuch3,4, Kasper Hornbæk4

m.obrist@sussex.ac.uk | a.tuch@unibas.ch | kash@diku.dk

1Culture Lab, School of Computing Science

Newcastle University, UK

2School of Engineering and Informatics

University of Sussex, UK

3Department of Psychology,

University of Basel, CH

4Department of Computer Science,

University of Copenhagen, DK

Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or

classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed

for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full

citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others

than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise,

or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific

permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from Permissions@acm.org.

CHI 2014, April 26 – May 01 2014, Toronto, ON, Canada

Copyright 2014 ACM 978-1-4503-2473-1/14/04…$15.00.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2556288.2557008
ABSTRACT

Technologies for capturing and generating smell are

emerging, and our ability to engineer such technologies and

use them in HCI is rapidly developing. Our understanding

of how these technologies match the experiences with smell

that people have or want to have is surprisingly limited. We

therefore investigated the experience of smell and the

emotions that accompany it. We collected stories from 439

participants who described personally memorable smell

experiences in an online questionnaire. Based on the stories

we developed 10 categories of smell experience. We

explored the implications of the categories for smellenhanced

technology design by (a) probing participants to

envision technologies that match their smell story and (b)

having HCI researchers brainstorm technologies using the

categories as design stimuli. We discuss how our findings

can benefit research on personal memories, momentary and

first time experiences, and wellbeing.

Author Keywords

Smell; smell experiences; odor; olfaction; user experience;

smell-enhanced technology; narratives; smell stories;

crowdsourcing; design brainstorming; designing for smell.

ACM Classification Keywords

H.5.2 Information interfaces and presentation (e.g., HCI):

Miscellaneous.

General Terms

Experimentation, Human Factors, Design.

INTRODUCTION

Smell plays an important role for memories and emotions.

Compared to other modalities, memories evoked by smell

give stronger feelings of being brought back in time, are

more emotionally loaded, are experienced more vividly,

feel more pleasant, and are autobiographically older

(ranging back to childhood) [15,33]. Smell is incredibly

powerful in connecting humans to past events and

experiences.

Matsukura et al. [22] recently proposed the Smelling

Screen, an olfactory display system that can distribute

smells. Earlier work in HCI has proposed other systems that

capture and generate smells. For example, Brewster et al.

[5] developed a smell-based photo-tagging tool, and Bodnar

et al. [4] showed smell to be a less disruptive notification

mechanism than visual and auditory modalities.

Thus, smell technologies are already emerging.

Our understanding of how these technologies match the

experiences with smell that people have or want to have is

surprisingly limited.

First, while technologies such as those

mentioned above are often evaluated, the results mainly

concern the perception of smell. The evaluations say little

about the general potential of smell technologies for

humans or their ability to generate particular experiences.

Second, whereas earlier work states that the subjective

experience of smell stimulation is crucial for the success of

a system (e.g., [5]), we are unaware of work in HCI that

studies the subjective experience of smell (though see [17]).

Third, several hundred receptors exist for smell and we

cannot rely on any primary smells to stimulate a particular

experience, as might be imagined for other human senses

[2].

Taken together, these points suggest that we can only

link smell tenuously to particular experiences or emotions.

This limits our ability to design for a spectrum of

experiences.

 

The present paper focuses instead on experiences and

emotions related to smell and links them to potential

technologies. Inspired by work on user experience [14,34],

we concentrate on personal memorable smell experiences

and their links to emotion. From the focus on experience we

developed design guidance for smell-enhanced

technologies. The goal is to contribute knowledge on

subjective smell experiences and their potential for design.

 

We collected 439 smell stories, that is, descriptions of

personal memorable experiences involving smell.We

distributed a questionnaire through crowdsourcing, ensuring

a large-scale coverage and variety of smell stories. We

analyzed the stories and identified 10 main categories and

36 sub-categories. Each category was described with

respect to its experiential and emotional characteristics and

specific smell qualities. Besides smell stories associated

with the past (e.g., memory of loved people, places, life

events) we identify stories where smell played an important

role in stimulating action, creating expectations, and

supporting change (e.g., of behavior, attitude, mood). Smell

can sometimes also be invasive and overwhelming, and can

affect people’s interaction and communication. Within the

categories, we identify common smell qualities and

emotions, which support the exploration of opportunities

for design. In particular, we discuss the implications for

technology based on feedback from participants and on a

brainstorming session with HCI researchers working on

smell technologies.

The main contributions of this paper are

(1) an experiencefocused understanding of smell experiences grounded in a

large sample of personal smell stories, which allowed us

(2) to establish a systematic categorization and description scheme for smell experiences, leading to

(3) the identification of technology implications by participants,and

(4) the exploration of design potentialities by HCI

researchers.

THE HUMAN SENSE OF SMELL

The sense of smell is the most complex and challenging

human sense.

Hundreds of receptors for smell exist and the

mixing of the sensations, in particular with our sense of

taste, is immense [2]. The sense of smell is further

influenced by other senses such as vision, hearing, and

touch; plays a significant role for memory and emotion; and

shows strong subjective preferences. Willander and Larsson

[33] showed that autobiographical memories triggered by

smell were older (mostly from the first decade of life) than

memories associated with verbal and visual cues (mostly

from early adulthood). Moreover, smell-evoked memories

are associated with stronger feelings of being brought back

in time, are more emotionally loaded, and are experienced

more vividly than memories elicited through other

modalities [15,33]. No other sensory system makes the

direct and intense contact with the neural substrates of

emotion and memory, which may explain why smellevoked

memories are usually emotionally potent [15].

The emotion-eliciting effect of smell is not restricted to the

context of autobiographical memories. Smell is particularly

useful in inducing mood changes because they are almost

always experienced clearly as either pleasant or unpleasant

[8]. For instance, Alaoui-Ismaïli et al. [1] used ‘vanilla’ and

‘menthol’ smells to trigger positive emotions in their

participants (mainly happiness and surprise) and ‘methyl

methacrylate’ and ‘propionic acid’ to trigger negative

emotions (mainly disgust and anger). Interestingly, Herz

and Engen [15] pointed out that almost all responses to

smell are based on associative learning principles. They

argued that only smells learned to be positive or negative

can elicit the corresponding hedonic response and that

people, therefore, should not have any hedonic preference

for novel smells. The only exceptions are smells of

irritating quality that strongly stimulate intranasal

trigeminal structures. Such smells often indicate toxicity.

While neuroscientists and psychologists have established a

detailed understanding of the human sense of smell, insight

into the subjective characteristics of smell and related

experiences is lacking.

The exploration of this subjective layer of smell is often understood as going beyond the

interest of these disciplines, but is highly relevant for HCI

and user experience research.

SMELL IN HUMAN-COMPUTER INTERACTION

Ten years ago, Kaye [17] encouraged the HCI community

to think about particular topics that need to be studied and

understood about smell. While some attempts have been

made to explore smell during recent years, the potential of

smell in HCI remains underexplored.

Most work on smell in HCI focuses on developing and

evaluating smell-enhanced technologies.

Brewster et al. [5]

used smell to elicit memories, and developed a smell-based

photo-tagging tool (Olfoto). Bodnar et al. [4] showed smell

to be less disruptive as a notification mechanism than visual

and auditory modalities. Emsenhuber et al. [9] discussed

scent marketing, highlighting the technological challenges

for HCI and pervasive technologies. Ranasinghe et al. [24]

further investigated the use of smell for digital

communication, enabling the sharing of smell over the

Internet. More examples of smell-enhanced technologies

can be found in multimedia applications [13], games [16],

online search interfaces [19], health and wellbeing tools

(e.g., http://www.myode.org/), and ambient displays [22].

The exploration of smell-enhanced technologies is mostly

limited to development efforts and the evaluation of users’

smell perception of single smell stimuli. The smells used

are often arbitrary and not related to experiences. This is

because of the lack of knowledge pertaining to the

description and classification of smells required for HCI

[17]. Kaye points out that “There are specific ones

[classification and description schemes] for the perfume,

wine and beer industries, for example, but these do not

apply to the wide range of smells that we might want to use

in a user interface” (p. 653). Thus, previous work has a

general and quite simple usage of smell.

 

THE POTENTIAL OF STUDYING SMELL EXPERIENCES

In contrast to the work reported above, the present paper

focuses instead on experiences with smell and links them to

potential technologies. We do so through stories of

experiences with smell. Stories are increasingly used within

user experience research to explore personal memories of

past experiences, but also to facilitate communication in a

design process [3,34]. Stories are concrete accounts of

particular people and events, in specific situations [10],

and

are more likely to stimulate empathy and inspire design thinking than, for example, scenarios.

STUDY METHOD

We asked a large sample of participants to report smell

experiences that were personal and meaningful.

We refer to the description of these experiences as smell stories. These

stories were captured through a questionnaire described

below, which included inspirational examples of smellenhanced

technologies at its end. Based on the examples we

asked participants to reflect on their experience and future

technologies. The rationale of this approach was to begin

from smell experiences that matter to participants, instead

of starting from an application or a particular technology.

Questionnaire

We created a web-based questionnaire consisting of six

parts. We started with an open question to stimulate the

report of a personal memorable smell experience. This was

followed by closed questions aiming to elucidate the

relevant emotional and experiential characteristics, as well

as the smell qualities. Participants could freely choose the

story to report. The questionnaire was administered through

a crowdsourcing platform to obtain a large sample of smell

stories. Crowdsourcing provides valid and reliable data [20]

and has been used for capturing user experiences [31].

Part 1: Smell Story

The smell stories were elicited through an initial exercise,

where participants were asked to think about situations and

experiences where smell played an important role. The aim

was to get participants into the right frame of mind and

sensitize them to smell. Next, participants were asked to

describe one memorable smell experience in as much detail

as possible, inspired by the questioning approach used in

explicitation interviews [23]. This questioning technique is

used to reconstruct a particular moment and aims to place a

person back in a situation to relive and recount it. Part 1 of

the survey was introduced as follows: Bring to your mind

one particular memorable moment of a personal smell

experience. The experience can be negative or positive.

Please try to describe this particular smell experience in as

much detail as possible. You can use as many sentences as

you like, so we can easily understand why this moment is a

memorable experience involving smell for you.

Participants were asked to give a title to their story

(reflecting its meaning) and indicate if the experience was

positive, negative, or ambivalent (i.e., equally positive and

negative). They were also asked to indicate how personally

relevant the experience was (from ‘not personally relevant

at all’ to ‘very personally relevant’).

Part 2: Context

Part 2 asked participants to give further details of their

reported experience via open and closed follow-up

questions. There were four questions on the context of the

described experience, including the social context (who else

was present), the place (based on the categories used by

[26]), the location (as an open field), and the time when the

reported experience took place (days, weeks, months, or

years ago).

Part 3: The smell

Specific questions on the characteristics and qualities of the

smell were asked in Part 3. Participants characterized the

smell itself using a list of 72 adjectives (i.e., affective and

qualitative terms) derived from the ‘Geneva Emotion and

Odor Scale’ (GEOS) [7]. Participants could also add

descriptions to characterize the smell in an open feedback

box. In addition, they rated the smell with respect to its

perceived pleasantness, intensity, and familiarity.

Part 4: Experienced emotions

In Part 4 participants had to describe how they felt about

the experience as a whole, using a list of affective terms

(101 in total). They could go through the list and tick the

words that best described their emotions during the

experience. The words were derived from Scherer [27].

Participants could also add their own words in a free-text

field.

Part 5: Smell technologies

After the participants had selected, thought about, and

described a particular smell episode, Part 5 linked their

personal experience to technology. The participants were

engaged in a envisioning exercise inspired by work on

mental time travel [30]. They were shown six inspirational

examples of smell technologies, namely: Olfoto: searching

and tagging pictures (CHI, [5]); Smelling screen: ambient

displays (IEEE, [22]); Digital smell: Sharing smell over the

Web (ICST, [24]); Scent dress: interactive fabric with smell

stimulation (http://www.smartsecondskin.com/); Mobile

smell App: iPhone To Detect Bad Breath and Other Smells

(BusinessInsider 01/2013), and Smell-enhanced cinema:

Iron Man 3 Smell-Enhanced Screening (Wired 04/2013).

These six technologies cover areas of relevance for HCI

(mobile, ambient, wearable, personal, and entertainment

computing), give realistic examples of smell technologies

from research, and include recent, commercial examples.

We asked the participants to imagine any desirable change

that future smell technology might make (or not) with

respect to their personal smell experience.

We asked them the following questions: (1) How could your experience be

enhanced? (2) What technology are you thinking about? (3)

Why would such a combination of your experience and the

technology be desirable, or why would it not?

Finally, the participants could express any other ideas for smell

technology in a free-text field.

Part 6: Personal background

At the end of the questionnaire, participants answered

questions on their socio-demographic and cultural

background. The goal was to try to identify any

geographical and cultural influences on smell attitudes (as

found by Seo et al. [29]). The participants were also asked

to assess their own smell sensitivity.

All the questions, except for those on demographics, were

mandatory. On average, the survey took 16 minutes to

complete (SD = 7.57 minutes). Participants received US$

1.50 for completing the questionnaire, corresponding to an

hourly salary of 5.63 dollars.

Collected data and participants

A total of 554 participants began the questionnaire. Of

these, 480 completed the questionnaire and answered three

verification questions at its end. These questions required

participants to describe the purpose of the study without

being able to go back and look at the earlier questions or

guidelines. After data cleaning, 41 stories were excluded.

Fake entries (n = 11) were identified immediately, while

repeated entries (n = 10), incomplete stories (unfinished

sentences; n = 6), and incomprehensible stories (which did

not make sense on their own; n = 14), were excluded

iteratively throughout the coding process. This left us with

439 smell stories.

At the time of the study, all 439 participants (52.8% female)

lived in the US; most had grown up in the US (95%). The

participants’ age ranged from 18 to 67 years (M = 31.5, SD

= 10.0). A majority of participants (84%) indicated being

sensitive to smell (rating 4 or higher on a scale from 1 to 5).

Data analysis

The analysis process followed an open and exploratory

coding approach [25]. Two researchers conducted the

qualitative coding process. After coding an initial 25% of

the stories, two more coding rounds (to reach 33% and then

50% of the data), led to the establishment of an agreed

coding scheme. The coding scheme contained 10 main

categories and 36 sub-categories, and a category entitled

‘not meaningful’ for cases where smell did not seem to

have any relevance in the described experience. Based on

this coding scheme, one researcher coded the remaining

50% of the data, and the second researcher coded a subsample

of 25% of that data, resulting in a good inter-coder

agreement (Cohen’s kappa of κ = .68) [12].

Follow up design brainstorming

In addition to the feedback from our participants,

we also explored the design value of the smell stories with experts

in the field. We organized a two-hour design brainstorming

session with three HCI researchers, two working on smell

technologies and one working on advanced interface and

hardware design. None of them were from the same

organization as the authors and none were familiar with the

details of the study before the session.

The brainstorming session aimed to share and interpret the

smell stories and followed four stages [11]: (1) prompting,

(2) sharing, (3) selecting, and (4) committing. We selected

36 stories (one representative story for each sub category)

as brainstorming prompts. All 36 stories were printed on A6

sheets (including the story title, the smell story, context

information, and personal background). Each researcher

was asked to read through the stories individually before

discussing them together. They were asked the same

questions as our participants (e.g., how they might imagine

a connection between the experience and technology). Each

researcher chose the most interesting/inspiring stories to

share with the group, then they generated ideas as a group,

and selected three to four ideas to be developed in more

detail. The outcome of the brainstorming session is

presented in the implication section, after the description of

the findings from the smell stories.

FINDINGS ON EXPERIENCES WITH SMELL

In the following sections we present our findings according

to the 10 identified categories. The 439 smell stories were

organized via their primary category, as agreed by the

coders. This categorization does not define a strict line

between the categories, as they are not wholly independent,

but it does enable us to organize the material and generate a

useful dataset for design.

Below we provide for each category a rich description of the particularities of the

stories, excerpts from example stories, and their associated

smell qualities and emotions. Each category also contains

information about the participants’ own rating of the stories

as positive, negative, or ambivalent.

Category 1: Associating the past with a smell

This category is the largest and contains 157 stories. In

these stories, the participants described a past experience in

which a smell was encountered during a special event in life

(e.g., ‘Wedding Day’, ‘New House’), at a special location

(e.g., ‘The Smells of Paris’, ‘Grandma’s House’), or as part

of a tradition (e.g., ‘The Smell of Thanksgiving’ or

‘Christmas Eve’). In these stories the smell was described

as having a strong association to those particular moments

in the past, with no actual smell stimulus in the present. A

particularity of this category is the distinction between

stories describing personal memorable events versus

personal life events (e.g., ‘Disneyland’ versus ‘When my

mother died’). Smells were also associated with personal

achievement/success (e.g., ‘Scent of Published Book’,

‘New Car Smell’) and other important episodes of change,

such as “‘Fresh Start’: I was taking a job in a new city. …. I

took a plane trip across the country and the moment I took

a step off the plane and took a deep breath will always stick

with me. It felt so clean and the air actually smelled fresh

and new” [#488]. Within this story, the qualities of the

smell were for instance described as fresh, energetic, and

invigorating. Some of the emotions experienced at this

moment were courageousness and excitement. Although

this category is dominated by positive experiences (n =

127), negative experiences were also reported (n = 27),

such as ‘Car Crash’.

Category 2: Remembering through a smell

The 40 stories in this category described a recent

experience of a smell, which reminded participants about

past events, people, locations, or specific times in their life.

In contrast to the previous category (where stories describe

a direct link from the recollected past smell to the present;

e.g., the smell of ‘Grandma’s House’), this category

contains stories that describe an indirect link from the

present experienced smell stimulus

to the past event, person or place (e.g., the smell of chocolate cookies as sudden

reminder about grandma). Most stories in this category

contain reminders of childhood described as ‘sweet’,

‘reassuring’ and ‘nostalgic’ with respect to the qualities of

the smell. A sub-set of stories in this category (n = 10) also

highlight the particular power of smells to take a person

back in time. The description of such a flashback caused by

a sudden smell stimulus was described as: “‘My first love’:

It was the next day, when I was walking through the local

Macy’s that I smelled something that threw me back into

that situation, I could feel and see everything that had

happened the day before when I smelled a perfume in the

store” [#630]. Some of the qualities used to describe the

smell were attractive, erotic, and fresh. The experienced

emotions were described as amorous, aroused, excited,

hopeful, and interested. The stories in this category were

mainly positive (n = 37), except for three.

Category 3: Smell perceived as stimulating

The 62 stories in this category described experiences with a

unique, mostly unknown smell (all stories, except one, were

positive). The smells arose from different sources, such as

perfume, food, and nature. A particularity of this category is

the quality of ‘first time’ encounters with a smell across all

origins. One participant described the first time he was at a

beach: “The smell was very different from anything I had

ever experienced before. At first I was kind of grossed out

by the smell, but I grew to love it” [#921]. Another

participant described the smell of a tornado experienced for

the first time: “It was similar to the smell before rain but

had a certain sharpness to it, as if to warn of the incoming

danger. I felt like I knew this smell but at the same time, it

felt foreign to me. It wasn’t a bad smell, it was just slightly

unfamiliar” [#713]. The smell qualities and experienced

emotions were often described with mixed attributes (e.g.,

heavy, imitating, and stimulating; attentive, serious, and

calm), but still rated as positive experiences by participants.

Most of the other stories in this category reported on the

first experiences with food (e.g., ‘Slice of Heaven’) and

nature (e.g., ‘Grass’), and were described as desirable,

fresh, or pure, and provoked feelings of happiness at the

moment they occurred. Although specific memories were

established, including unique new associations (e.g.,

‘Tornado smell’), the stories in this category did not evoke

the kind of strong connections to the past as described in

Category 1 and 2.

Category 4: Smell creating desire for more

This category contains 48 stories (45 positive). Key to these

stories is that the smell grabbed the persons’ attention

unexpectedly. The smell was either associated with food

(triggering appetite), nature (triggering curiosity), or the

scent of other people (triggering attractiveness), which

motivated one to do or get something. In some stories smell

was described in relation to the sensation of newness (e.g.,

“‘The sweet smell of CPU’: …There was the smell of the

cardboard boxes it all arrived in, the smell of new metal–

perhaps it was a combination of these and other things, but

when the building was complete there was just a singular

smell that was unique to a new computer built by my own

hands” [#685]). The qualities of the smell in this story

included beneficial, heavy, sophisticated, energetic, and

pleasantly surprising. The experienced excitement was

expressed through words such as confident, delighted,

enthusiastic, impressed, or triumphant. This category also

contained one story where the smell at a funeral stimulated

reflection in the moment (e.g., ‘The scent of moving on’).

The story was rated as a positive experience and at the same

time the smell was described as clean, penetrating, and

persistent, and the participant indicated that she was afraid,

anxious, discontented, sad, tired, and uncomfortable.

Despite the negative situation described in this story, the

smell gave hope and a desire to live and move on, looking

into the future in contrast to the stories in Category 1 and 2.

Category 5: Smell allowing identification and detection

This category captures the enabling role of smell in certain

situations, such as allowing one to identify or detect a smell

(e.g., “‘Gas leak’: I was cooking something on a gas stove

and went out for a few minutes. When I came back, the fire

was extinguished but the gas was still on. My roommate

was sat at the table doing schoolwork, completely oblivious

to the poisonous gas that was filling the room. I told him to

get the hell up and open the windows and doors” [#951]).

The qualities used to describe the smell were distinguished,

penetrating, dirty, and light. The emotions related to this

situation were described as anxious, conscientious,

confident, and serious. Although the category is rather

small (n = 11), the lesson to be learnt from the shared

stories was the immediacy of the smell, allowing the

participant to act or prevent something.

Category 6: Overwhelming power of smell

This category includes 37 stories where the smell

overwhelmed the person in a positive way (n = 5; e.g., ‘The

Chocolate Factory’) but predominately in a negative way (n

= 30; e.g., ‘The Smell of Death’). In the latter case, people

described the smell as something disturbing, as something

that hit them suddenly on their way or during an activity. A

subset of the stories was recounted as traumatizing, so that

the person could still vividly remember the particular

moment in the past although years have passed and no

recent similar smell stimulation had occurred unlike in

Category 2 (e.g., “‘Visit to a local county jail’: My guide

warned me ahead of the time that it was going to be a little

foul in there, but nothing could have prepared me for the

obscenely acrid stench of hundreds of men crammed into

every available space of the jail, right down to windowless

storage rooms converted into more cells. … For days

afterwards, I couldn’t shake the smell…. There weren’t

enough showers to take it away. It’s been several years

since then, and my memory of that smell is just a strong as

ever” [#604]). In this category, the qualities of the smell

were described as heavy, penetrating, dirty, or sickening.

Amongst others, the experienced emotions were described

as alarmed, anxious, distressed, frustrated, or

uncomfortable. In contrast to Category 1 and 2 (where the

smell was associated with an event from the past or

triggered a specific memory), Category 6 is about the smell

as such during the experience and not about the memory

associated with this smell. As opposed to the first two

categories, in most stories forgetting – not remembering –

the smell was the key element.

Category 7: Smell invading private and public spaces

All the stories in this category (n = 32) described an

experience where one could not get rid of the smell. The

smell invaded private and public spaces. In contrast to the

previous category, the smell entered the person’s personal

space (the person did not enter the space where the smell

already existed) and took over the space. The loss of control

over the smell was linked to the lingering quality of the

smell (e.g., “‘Don’t want to smell that twice!’: I woke up

one morning suddenly confused and was hit with an odor so

horrible I couldn’t figure out what it was. … It was not like

the smell you get a whiff of when a skunk stinks up the

outdoors” [#530]). In the story the power of the smell,

causing them to leave the house for several hours, was

described with qualities such as foul, nauseous, penetrating,

and persistent. One of the experienced emotions was

surprisingly ‘amused’, however it was overruled by other

emotions including annoyed, anxious, disgusted, taken

aback, and uncomfortable. Despite the glimpse of humor in

some stories, this category mainly contains negative

experiences and underlines the power of the smell with its

sudden and lingering qualities.

Category 8: Social interaction is affected by the smell

Within this category, smell was related to a person’s own

smell or to the smell of others. Smell negatively affected

the interaction among people and their togetherness (e.g.,

“‘Dragon breath teacher’: Once a teacher yelled at me

during class. She got so close up into my face that I could

smell her bad breath. This made the experience much worse

because I wanted to get up and walk away but she was

grabbing me to keep me focused on her while she was

talking” [#744]). The smell qualities were described as

nauseous, penetrating, and sickening, and caused negative

emotions experienced as bitter, distressed, or insulted.

Despite frequent interactions among people, this category

only contains 11 stories. This set of stories (overall negative

experiences, apart from two) contains interesting elements

with respect to a person’s own awareness of body smell and

the overbearing effect of other peoples’ smell on one’s

comfort.

Category 9: Smell changes mood, attitude and behavior

This category contains 23 stories, which underlined the

power of smells to change a person’s mood, attitude, or

behavior. Stories reported the active regulating effect of

smells with respect to mood, but mostly (n = 14) the change

of behavior due to smells (e.g., ‘Accidental vegetarian’ or

‘Saved by the Smell!’). One story showed the active usage

of smells to change one’s mood. A participant had recently

been divorced and reported on the day her husband had

moved out: “‘White Lilac Sheets’: “I made the bed with my

lilac sheets and the atmosphere changed. I still remember

that scent and how I felt on that day. I was going to be

okay. The room didn’t look or feel or smell lonely anymore.

It looked and smelled fresh and clean and lovely and a bit

romantic and it was mine” [#526]. The qualities of the

smell were described as fresh, reassuring, and spring-like,

while the experienced emotions were determined, hopeful,

longing, tense, but also worried. Overall, the stories in this

category were reported as mainly positive (n = 12)

experiences, but also as negative (n = 7) and four stories

were rated ambivalent, neither positive nor negative.

Category 10: Smell builds up and changes expectations

This category shows the potential of smell to build up

expectations and to surprise. In the former case (11 stories)

the smell was building up expectations until the actual

contact with the trigger, such as food or a perfume (e.g.,

“‘The Smell of Hungry Anticipation’: “I was trying a new

soup for the first time. When it was brought to the table, the

soft smell of rosemary immediately hit my nostrils. …It

complimented the taste of the soup and built anticipation”

[#585]). The smell was described as mouthwatering,

healthy, and pleasantly surprising, and was further related

to emotions such as conscientious, expectant, and relaxed.

In other stories (n = 7), expectations were exceeded to the

extent that they surprised and diverted anticipations (e.g.,

‘PomVinegar Surprise’: “I could smell the pomegranate

and vinegar from about 10 steps away, and it was a very

pungent (thought not unpleasant) odor. I almost felt my

nose becoming runny and took out a tissue. When I tasted

the dish, however, the taste wasn’t nearly as sour as I

expected it to be from the smell” [#542]). The smell in this

story was described as distinguished and penetrating, and

was associated with emotions such as attentive and excited.

Key quantitative facts behind the smell stories

While the above-described categories can be used as an

inspiration and as a starting point for exploring design

opportunities for smell in HCI,

our quantitative data provides additional background information. Below, the

key quantitative information across all the collected smell

stories is summarized. The majority of the 439 collected

stories were positively valenced (n = 296), 112 were

negative, and 31 were ambivalent. On average, negative

stories tended to be slightly longer (M = 90 words) than

positive stories (M = 79 words), but the difference is

statistically not significant (U = 14600, p = .063, r = .09).

Contextual information

Most stories occurred in a context where one or more familiar persons were present (64.2%)

or where participants were alone (21.6%). The presence of

one or more strangers was reported less frequently (8.7%).

With regard to location, most of the experiences happened

at the participant’s or a friend’s home (38.1%) or in a public

building (20.7%). Quite a few participants reported that

their experience took place in the streets or another public

space (14.4%), in a natural setting (7.3%), or at work

(6.4%). The remaining participants (13.2%) indicated other

places (e.g., stranger’s home). On average the reported

experiences occurred 8.7 years ago (SD = 10.3), ranging

from 1 day to 58 years ago.

The qualities of smell

The most frequent smell qualities

reported in positive stories were pleasant (60%), fresh

(42%), sweet (38%), clean (31%), and mouthwatering

(30%). Smells in negative stories were described as

unpleasant (62%), penetrating (55%), heavy (54%), foul

(53%), and nauseous (51%). In ambivalent stories the smell

was perceived as fresh (39%), pleasant (32%), mouthwatering

(32%), attractive (26%), and penetrating (23%).

Experienced emotions

When asked to describe how they felt during their experience, participants’ used the affective

terms happy (63%), pleased (53%), joyous (42%), delighted

(41%), and excited (39%) in positive stories and

uncomfortable (55%), disgusted (51%), distressed (43%),

miserable (42%), and taken aback (29%) in negative stories.

Ambivalent experiences were most frequently described as

happy (42%), excited (39%), enthusiastic (35%), joyous

(32%), and serene (29%).

An overview of all 10 categories and 36 sub-categories

including qualitative and quantitative information

(including a full example for each sub-category, used in the

design brainstorming session) is provided as supplementary

material. All smell stories and related qualities of smell,

experienced emotions, and context information, are also

available at www.multisensory.info for further exploration.

IMPLICATIONS FOR TECHNOLOGY

This study focused first on experiences and second on the

implications for technology. This section turns to

technology. Below we summarize the feedback from the

participants on how technology would fit with their

experience, and describe input from a brainstorming session

with HCI researchers working on smell technologies and

advanced interface and hardware design, based on a sub-set

of the smell stories (one from each sub-category).

Ideas for technology from participants

Below we summarize the six areas and ideas for desirable

future smell technologies mentioned by participants in Part

5 of the questionnaire:

(1) To share smells with family/friends: allow one to

participate in a family event through remote smelling; share

smells of special moments such as the smell of a newborn

baby with distant relatives; share smells with people who

they know would appreciate it (such as through social

media); allow capturing and sharing of smells to create a

common understanding where you can’t explain it.

Participants also desired to be able to design and share new

smells from a personal database and create a personal smell

box/bottle.

(2) To support decision making: use smells for a quick

judgment in online shopping (like/dislike can be determined

easily); create smell profiles about holiday places and travel

destinations; smell match maker in dating websites for

allowing a better decision making about going on a date or

not with a person (smell enhanced profiles).

(3) To regulate mood actively/passively: smell to relive

good experiences whenever you want to get in a better

mood; to calm yourself down in stressful moments such as

in traffic jam or at work; as a reminder of past memories

you would have forgotten otherwise but that can cheer you

up when you feel depressed and life seems too difficult.

(4) To combine with other technologies and activities:

integrate smell into radio; combine smell with music such

as with ‘soundhound’ or ‘shazam’ apps; smell-enhanced

advertisement on TV (for food channels); enhance visits of

concerts, theater and performances with smell; allow underwater

smelling when diving.

(5) To combine with everyday objects: enhance wristband

with smell for keeping a preferred perfume lingering; have

special glasses to see and smell the beach; smell-enhanced

jewelry and clothes. One participant imagined her wedding

ring enhanced with the smell of that day.

(6) To make oneself and others aware about body smell: to

avoid embarrassing moments; provide invisible cues to a

person about her/his smell level; quick smell check after

sporting activities.

The first idea matches the experiences in Categories 1 and

2, where particular events/moments in life are associated

with a smell. The desire for capturing and sharing these

experiences enhanced with smell becomes prevalent and

suggests design implications for real-time smell-enhanced

technologies (e.g., mobile phone, photo or video camera).

The second idea can be linked to Category 5, allowing

people to identify and detect a smell. Moreover, smells are

seen as very powerful for supporting quick decision-making

(e.g., smell-enhanced website navigation and searching).

The third idea shows a direct link to Category 9 and the

potential of smell to change mood. Interestingly participants

whose story was in Category 1 or 2 were wishing for the

possibility to capture pleasant smells, for instance from

their childhood, and released to them in the present. This

desire for smell-enhanced technologies or products is also

apparent in the fourth and fifth ideas, where technology,

objects, or even activities can be enhanced through smells

from the past, or actual smells sourced through nature (e.g.,

diving in the ocean). Finally, the sixth idea is linked to

Category 8, aiming to avoid embarrassing moments in

social interactions.

Participants also expressed concerns about future smell

technologies. They were concerned about the possible

misuse of smell when sharing it through the Internet or

mobile phones (e.g., teasing people with smells, how to

trust a smell message), and about the potential manipulation

through smell (e.g., TV ads, online shopping). Some

participants were also afraid to get sick, catch an allergy, or

become addicted if they are exposed to chemical

stimulations from technology. Finally, one participant

raised the question of copyright and ownership of smells

(e.g., ‘can I share others’ smells?’).

Ideas for technology from HCI researchers

Below we outline the ideas that emerged from the two-hour

brainstorming session prompted by 36 smell stories (one

from each sub-category). Four groups of design ideas

emerged from this session and are described below:

(1) Smell-enhanced performance regulator: a technology

stimulating smell in order to structure the day, taking

activities and moods into account, and combining different

smells to avoid habituation (training and evaluation phase

needed). Smell as a reminder to take a break or as

motivation to keep going a bit longer to meet the deadline

[inspired by #526 ‘White Lilac Sheets’, Category 9].

(2) Autonomous smell agent: a technology spreading

ambient cues (e.g., a robot) to guide someone to a certain

place, to build up expectations, and motivate action. Smell

trails in the environment can also make hidden information

accessible, for instance, before entering a room (e.g., smell

warning: tense working atmosphere) [inspired by #801

‘Don’t forget to check your gas stove before you leave the

house’, Category 5].

(3) Reminder alert with smell: a technology to remind us

about important events, birthdays, and appointments.

Although we have reminders on mobile phones and

computers, they are often ignored, snoozed or in the worst

case forgotten about. A smell can provide a pleasant

reminder to say ‘it is time to call your mom’ by presenting

the smell of your favorite dish your mom makes for you.

On the other hand, if more critical, bad smells can be very

powerful as a reminder and are not easily ignored [inspired

by #530 ‘Don’t want to smell that twice’, Category 7].

(4) Smell-enhanced storytelling: a technology that

stimulates storytelling around a digitized version of an

incense stick. A stick was imagined with different layers,

representing smells related to a loved person who passed

away. When friends or family members come together, for

instance at an anniversary year, they can add new smells to

be shared in the group and thus trigger new stories about

the dead person. It is as if looking through a photo album,

telling the stories from the past, and using the smells as

anchor points for keeping the memory alive [inspired by

#672 ‘The Scent of Moving On’, Category 4].

We saw that the smell stories, even if they only provided

limited information (story, story title, context, gender, and

age), triggered vivid discussions, created empathy, and

stimulated the sharing of personal smell experiences. The

four ideas described above provide a starting point for

exploring smell in HCI. The categorization along with

additional background information on smell qualities and

experienced emotions (see supplementary material) can

inspire further explorations of smell technologies.

DISCUSSION

Our findings about experiences with smell in combination

with the ideas for technologies just presented show several

design opportunities for smell. Below we do not provide

solutions for smell-enhanced technology designs, rather we

illustrate where our findings might be relevant to stimulate

novel designs in existing areas of interest within HCI. We

see three anchor points for smell-enhanced technology.

First, the smell stories in Categories 1 and 2 suggest design

opportunities for remembering and recalling the past. Our

findings might enrich ongoing research on the design for

personal memories. Apart from enhancing research

supporting the capturing and sharing of personal

experiences (e.g., in family relationships [18]) through

smell, our findings support research to support people who

are living with memory loss (e.g., patients with dementia

[32]), where smell can play an important role in

remembering the past. An increasing body of research also

explores the potential of digital technologies to support our

memory in everyday tasks (e.g., reminder systems), to

recall past events and experiences (e.g., life-logging tools),

to design end-of-life technologies allowing reminiscence of

passed away people [21], and to record and reproduce

smells [35]. All this research shows the potential of smell to

enrich experiences, for instance by enhancing personal

memories such as photos or videos with smell. Based on

their study of a smell-based photo-tagging system, Brewster

et al. [5] stated that participants asked for personal smells to

be added. The information on how to classify such smells

was still missing; the present analysis allows us to relate

smell qualities to particular types of experiences.

When designing with smell, as for any memory-based

technology, access to such memories has to be considered

carefully to preserve their uniqueness. One participant

wrote: “I could see it being desirable in that it would allow

me to experience the scent whenever I want, but it’s kind of

a two edged sword in that experiencing that scent time and

time again will make it common place” [#513]. The power

of smell might not persist if always available, thus

participants suggested to either restrict the access and

retrieval of smells to special times (e.g., at ‘grandmas

birthday’) or to link them to a certain social setting (e.g.,

smelling only in company with ‘your sister’). This way the

uniqueness of the smell can be preserved.

Second, the stories in Categories 3 to 8 as well as 10 draw

the attention away from past memories and suggest design

opportunities for the present moment. Designing for in-situ

stimulation, the ability to capture and share smells in the

moment, and the capability to mask and neutralize bad

smells creates a vast space for smell interaction design. One

suggestion made by participants was the combination of

smell and social media, such as “An app that would allow

me to store smells, send smells, or attach smells to a picture

that I could post on social media or Instagram or

something”. This supports existing research on the delivery

of smells through the Internet [24]. We draw attention to

three additional design directions concerned with (1) first

time experiences with smell, (2) the power of smell to build

up expectations, and (3) the potential of designing for bad

smells. User experience designers put a lot of effort into

designing ‘out-of-the-box’ and first time experiences to

create positive experiences [13]. Our categorization not

only provides designers with rich descriptions of such first

time experiences, but also describes the related qualities of

smells in combination with descriptors of the experienced

emotions. This can be used to stimulate positive smellenhanced

experiences with technology, build up

expectations, and create anticipation as studied within

experience research [33]. Typically this anticipation stage is

influenced by a variety of aspects (e.g., advertisements,

product descriptions, accounts from existing customers).

Smell stories in Category 10 provide evidence for the

power of smell to build up expectations, create surprise by

exceeding anticipated experiences, and enhance momentary

experiences through capturing and sharing pleasant smells.

Categories 6 to 8 contain stories about bad smells, which

are wished to be neutralized or masked to change the

experience from something negative to something positive.

While the idea of outbalancing smells seems to be

desirable, the design brainstorming session stimulated a

discussion on the usage of bad smell in design, particularly

as part of the design idea (3) Reminder alert with smell.

Designing for bad smells might not seem appropriate at

first, but through intensity manipulation it can open up an

interesting space for design. Similar to a snooze function,

which slowly increases volume, smell stimulations could be

added to certain events (e.g., reminder for mother’s

birthday). Starting with a pleasant smell, it could turn

slowly into something unpleasant if you did not act.

Category 8 also contained stories recounting social

experiences with smell, where the smell of a person or of

other people caused embarrassment or discomfort. Despite

the importance and frequency of social contact in everyday

life, few such stories were shared. They might not seem

meaningful enough to be memorable or to be shared. Yet,

this set of stories holds potential for personal and social

smell-enhanced awareness systems, as well as for wearable

technologies, and smart fabrics. Technology could, as stated

by a participant, “…make the people in those settings feel

more comfortable if I interact with them… My holding my

nose could be insulting and impede communication.”

Third, the smell stories in Category 9 suggest design

opportunities reaching out to the future through positive

stimulation, with potential relevance for wellbeing and

behavior change research in HCI. The stories shared in this

category were about the power of smell to regulate mood,

change attitudes, and behavior. Designing for smell could

be combined with behavior change research in HCI (e.g.,

tools to support healthy nutrition and diet), and thought of

in relation to positive psychology and research on

wellbeing. Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi [28] suggested

that happiness can be learned and cultivated and that

positive psychology can help change how a person feels.

They point to the power of positive emotions for our health,

happiness and wholeness. We would suggest that our

findings add an understanding of the positive emotional

impact of smells that might be a valuable research strategy

in wellbeing research (e.g., for regulating mood).

Smell can have a regulating impact on a person’s mood and can, as in

one case explicitly reported, be used to regulate mood

(‘White Lilac Sheets’). The participant wrote, “I guess the

experience could have been enhanced by some kind of

mood moderator. Something that would have sensed my

sadness and filled the room or house with comforting

scents” [#526]. The participant pointed out that technology

would not change the situation to something more positive,

as it just was not a happy time at all, but that it could

support the sad moments in this transitional period of life.

Limitations

We would like to acknowledge three limitations of this

work. First, by using Amazon Mechanical Turk for

recruiting and asking participants to describe personal

relevant experiences, we were limited to the US and do not

know to what extent the smell stories are representative of

more general experiences with smell. We are aware about

cultural and geographical differences (as described by Seo

et al. [29]), which require further studies with a more

diverse group of participants. Second, collecting narratives

by means of an online questionnaire has an influence on

how people narrate their experience and deprives us of the

advantages of an interview situation where we can engage

in a dialogue with the participant to explore the meaning

behind the shared experience in more depth as described by

Bruner [6]. We tried to collect information beyond the

initial trigger of the shared smell stories in order to allow

the establishment of meaningful categorizations and the

creation of a basic understanding of experiences with smell

in HCI.

Third, our approach provides an overview on the

emerging field of smell-enhanced technologies. Future

studies will, we hope, lead to in-depth research into

experiences with smell inspired by our identified categories.

 

CONCLUSIONS

Despite interactive technologies increasingly disappearing

into our environment (in ubiquitous and mobile computing)

and becoming essential in everyday life, the senses used to

interact with technology are still limited.

We have discussed the opportunities for smell in HCI based on an

analysis of 439 smell stories. We identified 10 primary

categories for stories about experiences with smell, which

help discuss the potential implications for technology.

Implications were drawn from feedback from our

participants envisaging desired connections between their

own personal experience and future smell technology. The

implications for designing for smell were further enriched

through ideas from an initial brainstorming session with

HCI researchers. Our findings provide guidance for smell

enhanced technology design, not only giving a

categorization of the role of smell in personal experiences,

but also extracting the qualities of smell across the smell

stories and the experienced emotions. We argue that this

research enriches existing technology driven research on

smell in HCI and provides a fruitful starting point when

designing for experiences with smell.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work is supported by the Marie Curie IEF Action of

the European Union (FP7-PEOPLE-2010-IEF) and the

Swiss National Science Foundation (PBBSP1 144196). We

thank our participants and especially Annika Haas for her

valuable support in designing the supplementary material.

REFERENCES

1. Alaoui-Ismaïli, O., Robin, O., Rada, H., Dittmar, A.,

Vernet-Maury, E. Basic Emotions Evoked by Odorants:

Comparison Between Autonomic Responses and Self-

Evaluation. Physiology & Beh. 62, 4 (1997), 713–720.

2. Bakalar, N. Sensory science: Partners in flavour. Nature

486, 7403 (2012), S4–S5.

3. Baumeister, R.F., Newman, L.S. How Stories Make

Sense of Personal Experiences: Motives that Shape

Autobiographical Narratives. Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin 20, 6 (1994), 676–690.

4. Bodnar, A., Corbett, R., Nekrasovski, D. AROMA:

ambient awareness through olfaction in a messaging

application. Proc. ICMI, (2004), 183–190.

5. Brewster, S., McGookin, D., Miller, C. Olfoto:

designing a smell-based interaction. Proc. CHI (2006),

653–662.

6. Bruner, J. Life as narrative. Social Research: An

International Quarterly 71, 3 (2004), 691–710.

7. Chrea, C., Grandjean, D., Delplanque, S., Cayeux, I., Le

Calvé, B., Aymard, L., Velazco, MI., Sander, D.,

Scherer, KR. Mapping the Semantic Space for the

Subjective Experience of Emotional Responses to

Odors. Chem. Senses 34, 1 (2009), 49–62.

8. Ehrlichman, H., Bastone, L. The use of odour in the

study of emotion. In S. Van and G.H. Dodd, (Eds.),

Fragrance: The psychology and biology of perfume.

Elsevier, (1992), 143–159.

9. Emsenhuber, B. Scent Marketing: Making Olfactory

Advertising Pervasive. In J. Müller, F. Alt and D.

Michelis (Eds.), Pervasive Advertising. Springer,

(2011), 343–360.

10. Erickson, T. Design as storytelling. Interactions 3, 4

(1996), 30–35.

11. Faste, H., Rachmel, N., Essary, R., Sheehan, E.

Brainstorm, Chainstorm, Cheatstorm, Tweetstorm: new

ideation strategies for distributed HCI design. Proc. CHI

(2013), 1343–1352.

12. Fleiss, J.L., Levin, B., Paik, M.C. Statistical Methods

for Rates and Proportions. John Wiley & Sons, (2013).

13. Ghinea, G., Ademoye, O. The sweet smell of success:

Enhancing multimedia applications with olfaction.

TOMCCAP 8, 1 (2012), 2.

14. Hassenzahl, M. Experience Design: Technology for All

the Right Reasons. Syn. Lect. on HCI 3, 1 (2010), 1–95.

15. Herz, R.S., Engen, T. Odor memory: review and

analysis. Psy. Bulletin & Review. 3, 3 (1996), 300–313.

16. Jonsson, F., Verhagen, H. Senses working overtime: on

sensuous experiences and public computer game play.

Proc. ACE (2011), 56:1–56:8.

17. Kaye, J. “Jofish.” Making Scents: aromatic output for

HCI. Interactions 11, 1 (2004), 48–61.

18. Kazakos, K., Howard, S., Vetere, F. Revisiting the

relationship between reunion and technology-mediated

separation in periodically transitioning families. Proc.

CSCW (2013), 1157–1168.

19. Loumakis, F., Stumpf, S., Grayson, D. This image

smells good: effects of image information scent in

search engine results pages. CIKM (2011), 475–484.

20. Mason, W., Suri, S. Conducting behavioral research on

Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavior Research

Methods 44, 1 (2012), 1–23.

21. Massimi, M., Moncur, W., Odom, W., Banks, R., Kirk,

D. Memento mori: technology design for the end of life.

Proc. CHI EA (2012), 2759–2762.

22. Matsukura, H., Yoneda, T., Ishida, H. Smelling Screen:

Development and Evaluation of an Olfactory Display

System for Presenting a Virtual Odor Source. IEEE

TVCG 19, 4 (2013), 606–615.

23. Petitmengin, C. Describing one’s subjective experience

in the second person: An interview method for the

science of consciousness. Phenomenology and the

Cognitive Sciences 5, 3-4 (2006), 229–269.

24. Ranasinghe, N., Karunanayaka, K., Cheok, A.D.,

Fernando, O.N.N., Nii, H., Gopalakrishnakone, P.

Digital taste and smell communication. Proc. BodyNets,

(2011), 78–84.

25. Saldana, J. The Coding Manual for Qualitative

Researchers. SAGE (2012).

26. Scherer, K.R. Appraisal considered as a process of

multilevel sequential checking. Appraisal processes in

emotion: Theory, methods, research 92, (2001), 120.

27. Scherer, K.R. What are emotions? And how can they be

measured? Social Science Inf. 44, 4 (2005), 695–729.

28. Seligman, M.E.P., Csikszentmihalyi, M. Positive

psychology: An intro. Am. Psy. 55, 1(2000), 5–14.

29. Seo, H.-S., Guarneros, M., Hudson, R., et al. Attitudes

toward Olfaction: A Cross-regional Study. Chem.

Senses 36, 2 (2011), 177–187.

30. Suddendorf, T., Corballis, M.C. The evolution of

foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to

humans? Beh. & Brain Sciences 30, 3 (2007), 299–313.

31. Tuch, A.N., Trusell, R., Hornbæk, K. Analyzing users’

narratives to understand experience with interactive

products. Proc. CHI (2013), 2079–2088.

32. Wallace, J., Wright, P.C., McCarthy, J., Green, D.P.,

Thomas, J., Olivier, P. A design-led inquiry into

personhood in dementia. Proc. CHI (2013), 2617–2626.

33. Willander, J., Larsson, M. Smell your way back to

childhood: Autobiographical odor memory. Psy.

Bulletin & Review 13, 2 (2006), 240–244.

34. Wright, P., McCarthy, J. Experience-Centered Design:

Designers, Users, and Communities in Dialogue. Syn.

Lect. on HCI 3, 1 (2010), 1–123.

35. Wyszynski, B., Yamanaka, T., Nakamoto, T. Recording

and reproducing citrus flavors using odor recorder.

Sensors&Actuators B: Chemical 106, 1(2005), 388–393.